Volume 10, Issue 1 (Autumn 2024)                   Health in Emergencies and Disasters Quarterly 2024, 10(1): 49-58 | Back to browse issues page

Ethics code: IR.RCS.REC.1402.023


XML Print


Download citation:
BibTeX | RIS | EndNote | Medlars | ProCite | Reference Manager | RefWorks
Send citation to:

Fatahi Ardakani M, Zareipour M, Karimi Kiwi H, Yamani Ardakani B, Bahrevar V, Kazemi S. The Perception of Residents About the Performance of Local TV During the 2022 Flash Flood in Yazd, Iran. Health in Emergencies and Disasters Quarterly 2024; 10 (1) :49-58
URL: http://hdq.uswr.ac.ir/article-1-602-en.html
1- Department of Health Education, School of Health, Shahid Sadougi University of Medical Science, Yazd, Iran., Continuous Education, Shahid Sadoughi University of Medical Sciences, Yazd, Iran
2- Department of Public Health, School of Health, Khoy University of Medical Sciences, Khoy, Iran., Department of Public Health, Khoy University of Medical Sciences, Khoy, Iran
3- Applied Science Higher Education Institute, Red Crescent Society of the Islamic Republic of Iran, Tehran, Iran., Applied Science Higher Education Institute Red Crescent Society of the Islamic Republic of Iran, Tehran,Iran
4- Department of Health Education, School of Health, Shahid Sadougi University of Medical Science, Yazd, Iran., Shahid Sadougi University of medical science and health services,Yazd, Iran
5- Department of Health Education, School of Health, Shahid Sadougi University of Medical Science, Yazd, Iran., Department of Aging,University of social welfare and rehabilitation sciences, Teharn, Iran -Shahid Sadoughi University of medical sciences and health services,Yazd,Iran
6- Research Center for Emergency and Disaster Resilience, Red Crescent Society of the Islamic Republic of Iran, Fars, Iran. , info.sadeghkazemi@gmail.com
Full-Text [PDF 469 kb]   (187 Downloads)     |   Abstract (HTML)  (661 Views)
Full-Text:   (79 Views)
Introduction
atural disasters, such as earthquakes, floods, storms, dust phenomena, landslides, and droughts, annually cause high human and financial loses [1]. Floods are one of the most important natural disasters with meteorological and hydrological hazards [2]. Floods can destroy buildings and infrastructure [3]. They account for more than one-third of the damage caused by natural disasters in developed and developing countries [4]. According to the recent report of the intergovernmental panel on climate change (IPCC), the frequency and severity of floods will be increased due to continued climate change. Floods have affected human societies more than other natural disasters in the 21st century. In the last 18 years, since 2000, more than 121 million people have been affected by floods, ranking it first among the natural hazards worldwide. In terms of death rate, more than 8,000 people died by floods, putting it in the third rank [5]. Various studies have indicated that the lack of attention to the privacy of waterways and rivers has caused a dramatic increase in the frequency of floods and their damage [6, 7]. It is possible to reduce their harmful and destructive consequences with proper planning and perception of the facts related to floods [8]. In Iran, due to climatic and geographical conditions, floods occur almost every year and cause heavy damage. Although Iran is a dry country, more than half of its land is subject to floods [9]. On average, more than 170,000 people in Iran are affected by floods annually, and 242 people die [1]. 
Flash floods are the most dangerous kind of floods [10]. A flash flood can cause damage at any time and place without prior or partial warning [11]. Preparedness against floods, especially flash floods, requires officials and people to recognize and perceive the risks [12]. Risk perception is a subjective judgment of the likelihood of adverse events and the amount of future negative outcomes [13]. People’s perception of the risks can be very helpful in disaster management [14]. Risk perception is an important subjective concept. In general, two exogenous variables in risk perception include subjective evaluation of the probability of an unwanted or adverse event and judgment about the severity of the adverse event if it occurred [15, 16]. Therefore, assessment of risk probability and people’s judgment about the severity of risk is effective in people’s decisions and preventive measures [17, 18]. How people behave in an emergency situation depends on their perception of the risk and evaluation of the level of outcome. The negative consequences of disasters are related to the low risk perception [16]. Flash floods can be identified and understood easily and accurately by the general population through observation [19]. Without a proper risk perception assessment, it is not possible to understand people’s risk perceptions and reduce the risks [20]. There are significant differences between the perceptions of flood risk by experts compared to the perception of flood risk by people. Experts are more realistic in their approach to flood risk [21]. The perception of flood risk is an essential step for encouraging vulnerable people to engage in flood risk reduction [22]. Flash floods have recently occurred in many provinces of Iran [23, 24]. Despite the warnings of the Meteorological Organization about bad weather conditions and the possibility of flood, many people did not pay attention to these warnings and traveled to high-risk places [25]. Therefore, the knowledge of the factors that can help people understand the risk correctly is very decisive. 
Several studies have been conducted to investigate the flash flood risk perception of people, and different scales have been used to measure it [26-28]. Miceli et al. considered risk perception as a process with two cognitive (awareness, likelihood, etc.) and affective (feelings, perceived control, etc.) aspects [29]. Age, sex, proximity to the risk area, living near a river, awareness of floods, previous experience with floods, religion, perceived support from the authorities, media coverage, and trust in the media have been introduced as examples of factors affecting the perception of flood risk [4, 30, 31]. Mass media can greatly help with managing crises such as flash floods by broadcasting and increasing information and awareness [31-33]. In disaster-related studies, trust in the media has been identified as a significant factor in risk perception [31, 32, 34]. Understanding people’s experiences with floods, their ease of access to information, and media use can be beneficial in improving risk perception. Therefore, this study aims to explore the perceptions of people in Yazd, Iran, regarding the performance of TV programs in informing about flash floods in the city. This city is located in the center of Iran (latitude: 31.90°, longitude: 54.29°, and altitude: 1237 meters above sea level). The average temperature in this city is 19.8 °C, with a relative humidity of 31% and an annual precipitation of 46.2 mm [35].

Materials and Methods
This qualitative study was conducted using the directed content analysis method. The study population consists of all residents of Yazd City who had suffered from flash floods. Participants (including office workers, residents, and local vendors) were selected from among those who watched local TV programs and were affected by the flash floods. The sampling was done using a purposive method until reaching data saturation. Partticipants were selected based on their willingness to engage in the interviews and familiarity with local TV prorgams in Yazd. After explaining the study objectives and process to the participants and ensuring their confidentiality, a semi-structured interview was conducted to collect data. 
The first interview question was “Please describe your experience with flash floods”. After that, the interview continued based on the participants’ answers using exploratory questions such as “What do you mean?” and “Please elaborate on this”. There were also questions such as: “Do you watch local TV programs?”; “Did you receive any news about the flash flood? Can you explain more?”; “How much did you pay attention to the news on TV?”; “What is your opinion about the news reported on national or local TV programs? Can you explain more?” The interview questions also surveyed demographic characteristics of participants. The interview questions were sent to managers, professors, and experts in the field of disaster risk management, disaster health, and qualitative researchers to comment on their appropriateness. Their feedback was used to modify the questions. The interviews were conducted from December 2022 to February 2023 (seven months after the flood occurnce). Twenty-two participants were interviewed face to face in their place of residence. Each interview took 30-55 minutes. The interviews were recorded and transcribed verbatim.
Data analysis was done by Graneheim and Lundman’s method [36]. The interview texts were read several times and the statements related to the experiences of flash floods were extracted and semantic units were created. After summarizing and integrating semantic units, coding was done. The codes were categorized based on similarities and differences. Finally, themes and sub-themes were extracted. MAXQDA 10 software was used for qualitative data analysis. 
Lincoln and Guba’s criteria [37] were used to ensure the trustworthiness of the data. The researcher allocated enough time to collect and analyze the data and a good relationship was established with the participants. Data diversity was used to give more depth to the data. Continuous comparison of data and review of codes by the research team was also done to ensure the credibility of the data. To increase the dependability, the stages of qualitative analysis and the different stages of the content analysis method were written and followed in full detail. To increase the confirmability of the data, a summary of interview texts was given to the participants at the end of each interview so that the researcher’s correct perception of the interview can be confirmed. Also, during the data analysis and classification process, the extracted themes and codes were shown to some of the participants and their opinion was asked about their accuracy. To increase the transferability of the data, we tried to describe the culture, context, characteristics of the participants, the data collection method, and the data analysis method in full detail. 

Results
Most of the participants were in the age group of 19-68 years (Mean age: 41±1.5 years). Four participants had primary education, four had lower than high school education, one had middle school education, four had high school diploma, two had associate degree, four had bachelor’s degree, and three had master’s degree. Table 1 shows other demographic characteristics of the participants. 



Analyzing the data led to the emergence of one main concept (information quality), five themes (time of broadcasting news, content of programs, trust in the media, and ability to increase social sensitivity), 12 sub-themes, and 48 codes (Table 2). 



The first theme (time of broadcasting news) included two sub-themes of “devoting insufficient time to the topic” and “insufficient information”. Special attention to daily needs of each province is one of the missions of local TV programs. Participants stated that if there is a possibility of flash floods in the province, all authorities should cooperate with each other. One of the participants stated: “There was a flood in the province, but it was only broadcasted once” (participant (P) 11). Another participant stated, “If There is a flooding in the province, we expect the local TV to pay special attention to it and inform people about it at any time” (P14). Another participant stated: “During floods, they should give priority to it and not limit it to reporting in the news shows and let people hear and provide their opinions at any time” (P21). Also, participants stated that a local TV should provide up-to-date and detailed information to people (P3 and P10). 
The second theme (the content of news) included four sub-themes of “news with the same method of production,” “neglecting the type of news,” “message validity,” and “lack of innovation.” One of the reasons for not paying attention to local TV programs was the production of programs using the same method. Many participants perceived that the programs related to floods were like other programs. They want local TV to produce programs related to floods in different ways so that they can better emphasize the importance of the crisis. A 19-year-old lady said, “I watched TV several times the day before the flash flood, but I did not see news about the possibility of flooding. They reported it half an hour after the occurrence! Of course, flood management methods were not reported in this program” (P1). P16 stated, “I saw the flash flood news on Channel One. I also saw it on the local channel. Simple and incomplete news was broadcast several times during the flood. It was very poor”. Another sub-theme was the lack of attention to the type of news. One of the participants stated, “It is not possible to differentiate between flood news and other news. I think that they should pay more attention to floods” (P7). Another participant argued, “I was watching a local channel; the flood-related news was being reported among other news. I did not pay attention to it. It was like other news. They did not tell us about the actions needed to be taken” (P11). Poor expertise was included in the sub-themes related to the programs. A number of participants did not consider the experts’ statements to be very accurate. Therefore, they felt that the meteorologists were not so familiar with modern methods or perceived that the experts might not have expertise; otherwise, their predictions would be better. One of the participants stated, “I think the meteorologist gives conflicting opinions. I have heard several times that his statements do not match reality. Therefore, I don’t pay much attention to it” (P15). Another participant said, “I think the meteorologist provides information similar to what he said in the previous nights! I don’t pay much attention to him anymore, and I don’t consider his words important” (P4). Lack of innovation was the last sub-theme. Many participants stated that the news related to floods should be presented differently to increase the audience’s sensitivity. Participant No. 8 stated, “I didn’t understand this news well. If this news was told in a different way, maybe I would have understood it better”. Another participant expressed: “The TV only shows that clouds increase in a region and that a flood might come. It would be better if the presenter provided more details and more simply (P4). 
Trust in the media was the third theme with the following sub-themes: False news, weak proficiency, poor programs, and repeated programs on the local TV. One of the factors that can reduce trust in news or programs is not being close to reality and their weak predictions. The participants had experienced wrong news and predictions, especially in weather forecasting. A 21-year-old student stated his experiences as follows: “I don’t think the news related to the weather of the province is correct. Whenever they said that we would probably have rain in the province tomorrow, there was no rain, or the weather was favorable; there was only a dust weather! Do you think anyone can trust in this news?” (P18). A 33-year-old man argued: “I don’t trust in weather news much. Out of three pieces of news, one piece of news is fake. I believe a solution should be considered for it” (P16). Regarding the poor performance of local TV programs, a participant (P15) expressed that the quality of programs was not at a high level and that society did not pay special attention to the programs. If people feel that the local TV programs are produced with high quality, their views can be changed, and they will pay more attention (P20). Regarding the repeated TV programs, which are not attractive or are used only for free hours, one of the participants stated: “Every time I want to watch a local TV channel, I see the rebroadcast of TV series! If news is supposed to be presented at the provincial level, they should pay special attention to it on local TV. I am so disappointed with the local channel” (P7). P13 argued: “I expect that a crisis like flooding in the province should receive more attention and the performance of responsible organizations should be monitored and controlled; one of the provincial plans should be assigned to this topic. Whenever there was heavy rain in Yazd, floodwaters could be found on roads and bridges, and traffic would be disrupted for several days”. 
The final theme (i.e. Inability to increase social sensitivity) included 2 sub-themes of “showing the importance of news” and “content production in different ways”. One of the things that can affect the community is to create sensitivity to news. In this regard, due to the importance of flood-related news, the presentation method needs to be taken into account. Many participants did not perceive that the news related to the flood or possible rain could warn them. Therefore, they did not take any special measures to address the possible risks of flash floods. They stated that they were not so excited about the news and did not feel threatened. They stated that presenting the news with a different method and a better warning system would have had a greater impact. One of the participants said, “I watched the news, but it had no considerable impact on me” (P16). Participant No. 10 said, “I didn’t care much about the news. It would be better if it was broadcast in another way”. Another participant mentioned: “I watched the news related to the flood; it was like other news. If the consequences of the flood and prevention methods were added to the news, it would have a greater impact” (P17). 

Discussion
During flash floods and crisis management, there is a higher need for help from related organizations. One of the determining factors in risk perception is the news from reliable sources. One of these sources is the TV program. Considering the TV programs’ ability to increase the awareness and attention of people, it would be beneficial to broadcast flood news on TV [38]. Devoting enough time to the news can affect people’s risk perception. Providing insufficient information about floods by the local TV was one of the reasons for people’s lack of attention to this disaster. According to Bloom’s hierarchical model of learning, a person needs to be exposed to the message to pay attention to it. Therefore, wide coverage of news increases the probability of perceiving the message [38]. Many participants in this study reported that they do not pay special attention to the news and programs on the local TV. The mentioned reasons included the programs with the same production method, neglecting the type of news, message validity, and lack of innovation. Therefore, if special attention is paid to the content of programs and news, or if they the programs and news are matched to the needs of people and the conditions of the society, or are designed based on innovative educational and psychological methods, better results will be obtained. The content of news should catch the audience’s attention. It play an important role in changing the attitude and behavior of people towards the crisis [39]. One of the indicators of the effectiveness of educational content is its attractiveness to the audience [40]. Therefore, the programs should be designed based on the target groups and their interests. In this study, participants perceived local TV’s lack of attention to community needs and a lack of innovation in reporting the news. 
Lack of trust in the media was another theme extracted in this study, with sub-themes of false news, low proficiency in presenting news, poor programs, and broadcast of routine programs on the local TV. One of the influential factors related to media influence is media credibility, which refers to the believability of news or sources. The credibility issue had not received much attention from local TV, manifested in the form of using experts with low proficiency and lack of effort in producing high-quality programs, which is similar to the results of a recent study [30]. One of the problems mentioned by the participants was the broadcast of routine programs that led to a lack of interest in watching the news and paying attention to the program. In past studies, this issue has not been directly reported [41]. However, it is obvious that if new TV programs are produced based on people’s needs, they will develop a more positive attitude towards local TV, which can increase their attention to the programs or news. 
The inability to increase social sensitivity was the last theme. To create a proper risk perception while stimulating the risk level and creating perceived sensitivity, basic guidelines for reducing the level of risk are recommended [7]. Risk perception guidelines can help people take the flash flood risk more seriously. If they believe that the flash flood is a serious risk, the probability of proper responses and measures for crisis prevention and management increases [42]. 

Conclusion
Time of broadcasting news, content of programs, trust in the media, and ability to increase social sensitivity were extracted as the factors affecting the performance of local TV programs in informing about flash floods in Yazd. Reporting important news and warnings about flash floods on various channels, employing innovative production methods, and increase the pubic trust by using credible and accurate news can play a pivotal role in enhancing the performance of local TV in Yazd. Interviewing people about their experiences of an event depends on the importance of the topic to them. In addition, some people may not remember the details of the event. Due to this limitation, only those who had a good memory of floods were interviewed in this study.

Ethical Considerations

Compliance with ethical guidelines

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Education, Research, and Technology Division of the Iranian Red Crescent Society (Code: IR.RCS.REC.1402.023). A written consent form was obtained from the participants before the study. They all were free to leave the study at any time, and their information was kept confidential. 

Funding
This research did not receive any grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or non-profit sectors.

Authors' contributions
The authors equally contributed to preparing this article.

Conflict of interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank the people who participated in the study for their cooperation




References
  1. Smith KL, Kerr DA, Fenner AA, Straker LM. Adolescents just do not know what they want: A qualitative study to describe obese adolescents’ experiences of text messaging to support behavior change maintenance post intervention. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 2014; 16(4):e103. [DOI:10.2196/jmir.3113] [PMID] [PMCID]
  2. Ward PJ, Daniell J, Duncan M, Dunne A, Hananel C, Hochrainer-Stigler S, et al. Invited perspectives: A research agenda towards disaster risk management pathways in multi-(hazard-) risk assessment. Natural Hazards and Earth System Sciences. 2022; 22(4):1487-97. [DOI:10.5194/nhess-22-1487-2022]
  3. Bronstert A, Crisologo I, Heistermann M, Ozturk U, Vogel K, Wendi D. Flash-floods: More often, more severe, more damaging? An analysis of hydro-geo-environmental conditions and anthropogenic impacts. In: Leal Filho W, Nagy G, Borga M, Chávez Muñoz P, Magnuszewski A, editors. Climate change, hazards and adaptation options. Climate Change Management. Cham: Springer; 2020. [DOI:10.1007/978-3-030-37425-9_12]
  4. Lechowska E. What determines flood risk perception? A review of factors of flood risk perception and relations between its basic elements. Natural Hazards. 2018; 94(3):1341-66. [DOI:10.1007/s11069-018-3480-z]
  5. Mead EL, Cohen JE, Kennedy CE, Gallo J, Latkin CA. The influence of graphic warning labels on efficacy beliefs and risk perceptions: A qualitative study with low-income, urban smokers. Tobacco Induced Diseases. 2016; 14:25. [DOI:10.1186/s12971-016-0088-5] [PMID] [PMCID]
  6. Yari A, Zarezadeh Y, Ardalan A, Boubakran MS, Rahimiforoushani A, Bidarpoor F, et al. Deadly floods and their causal factors: A case-control study in Iran between 2005 and 2018. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction. 2022; 77:103036. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijdrr.2022.103036]
  7. Birkholz S, Muro M, Jeffrey P, Smith HM. Rethinking the relationship between flood risk perception and flood management. The Science of The Total Environment. 2014; 478:12-20. [DOI:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2014.01.061] [PMID]
  8. Lechowska E. Approaches in research on flood risk perception and their importance in flood risk management: a review. Natural Hazards. 2022; 111(3):2343-78. [DOI:10.1007/s11069-021-05140-7]
  9. Rana IA, Jamshed A, Younas ZI, Bhatti SS. Characterizing flood risk perception in urban communities of Pakistan. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction. 2020; 46:101624. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijdrr.2020.101624]
  10. Han Z, Sharif HO. Analysis of flood fatalities in the United States, 1959-2019. Water. 2021; 13(13):1871. [DOI:10.3390/w13131871]
  11. Maqtan R, Othman F, Wan Jaafar WZ, Sherif M, El-Shafie A. A scoping review of flash floods in Malaysia: Current status and the way forward. Natural Hazards. 2022; 114(3):2387-416. [DOI:10.1007/s11069-022-05486-6]
  12. Monteil C, Foulquier P, Defossez S, Péroche M, Vinet F. Rethinking the share of responsibilities in disaster preparedness to encourage individual preparedness for flash floods in urban areas. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction. 2022; 67:102663. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102663]
  13. Lundborg P, Lindgren B. Do they know what they are doing? Risk perceptions and smoking behaviour among Swedish teenagers. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 2004; 28:261-86. [DOI:10.1023/B:RISK.0000026098.84109.62]
  14. Baan PJ, Klijn F. Flood risk perception and implications for flood risk management in the Netherlands. International Journal of River Basin Management. 2004; 2(2):113-22. [DOI:10.1080/15715124.2004.9635226]
  15. Ferrer RA, Klein WM. Risk perceptions and health behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2015; 5:85-9. [DOI:10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.03.012] [PMID] [PMCID]
  16. Grothmann T, Reusswig F. People at risk of flooding: Why some residents take precautionary action while others do not. Natural Hazards. 2006; 38:101-20. [DOI:10.1007/s11069-005-8604-6]
  17. Sjöberg L, Moen B, Rundmo T. Explaining risk perception. An evaluation of the psychometric paradigm in risk perception research. Norwegian University of Science and Technology, C Rotunde Publikasjoner; 2004. [Link]
  18. Vasvári T. Risk, risk perception, risk management-a review of the literature. Public Finance Quarterly= Pénzügyi Szemle. 2015; 60(1):29-48. [Link]
  19. Hapuarachchi HAP, Wang QJ, Pagano TC. A review of advances in flash flood forecasting. Hydrological Processes. 2011; 25(18):2771-84. [DOI:10.1002/hyp.8040]
  20. Scott M, White I, Kuhlicke C, Steinführer A, Sultana P, Thompson P, et al. Living with flood risk/The more we know, the more we know we don’t know: Reflections on a decade of planning, flood risk management and false precision/Searching for resilience or building social capacities for flood risks?/Participatory floodplain management: Lessons from Bangladesh/Planning and retrofitting for floods: Insights from Australia/Neighbourhood design considerations in flood risk management/Flood risk management-Challenges to the effective implementation of a paradigm shift. Planning Theory & Practice. 2013; 14(1):103-40. [DOI:10.1080/14649357.2024.2322879]
  21. Babcicky P, Seebauer S. The two faces of social capital in private flood mitigation: Opposing effects on risk perception, self-efficacy and coping capacity. Journal of Risk Research. 2017; 20(8):1017-37. [DOI:10.1080/13669877.2016.1147489]
  22. Hajat S, Ebi K, Kovats R, Menne B, Edwards S, Haines A. The human health consequences of flooding in Europe: A review. In: Kirch W, Bertollini R, Menne B, editors. Extreme weather events and public health responses. Berlin: Springer; 2005. [DOI:10.1007/3-540-28862-7_18]
  23. Khosravi K, Pham BT, Chapi K, Shirzadi A, Shahabi H, Revhaug I, et al. A comparative assessment of decision trees algorithms for flash flood susceptibility modeling at Haraz Watershed, Northern Iran. Science of the Total Environment. 2018; 627:744-55. [DOI:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2018.01.266] [PMID]
  24. Ardalan A, Holakouie Naieni K, Kabir MJ, Zanganeh AM, Keshtkar AA, Honarvar MR, et al. Evaluation of Golestan province’s early warning system for flash floods, Iran, 2006-7. International Journal of Biometeorology.2009; 53(3):247-54. [DOI:10.1007/s00484-009-0210-y] [PMID]
  25. Safarpour H, Sarani M, Safi-Keykaleh M. Necessity of focus on the disaster risk perception: Lessons from the 2019 floods in Iran. Disaster Medicine and Public Health Preparedness. 2021; 15(2):135-6. [DOI:10.1017/dmp.2019.165] [PMID]
  26. Diakakis M, Priskos G, Skordoulis M. Public perception of flood risk in flash flood prone areas of Eastern Mediterranean: The case of Attica Region in Greece. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction. 2018; 28:404-13. [DOI:10.1016/j.ijdrr.2018.03.018]
  27. Ahmad D, Afzal M. Flood risk public perception in flash flood-prone areas of Punjab, Pakistan. Environmental Science and Pollution Research. 2022; 29(35):53691-703. [DOI:10.1007/s11356-022-19646-5] [PMID]
  28. Kellens W, Terpstra T, De Maeyer P. Perception and communication of flood risks: A systematic review of empirical research. Risk Analysis: An International Journal. 2013; 33(1):24-49. [DOI:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01844.x] [PMID]
  29. Miceli R, Sotgiu I, Settanni M. Disaster preparedness and perception of flood risk: A study in an alpine valley in Italy. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2008; 28(2):164-73. [DOI:10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.10.006]
  30. Martins B, Nunes A, Lourenço L, Velez-Castro F. Flash flood risk perception by the population of Mindelo, S. Vicente (Cape Verde). Water. 2019; 11(9):1895. [DOI:10.3390/w11091895]
  31. Knocke ET, Kolivras KN. Flash flood awareness in southwest Virginia. Risk Analysis: An International Journal. 2007; 27(1):155-69. [DOI:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2006.00866.x] [PMID]
  32. McPherson HJ, Saarinen TF. Flood plain dwellers’ perception of the flood hazard in Tucson, Arizona. The Annals of Regional Science. 1977; 11:25-40. [DOI:10.1007/BF01287852]
  33. O’Neill E. Understanding risk in environmental planning. In: Davoudi S, Cowell R, White I, Blanco H. The Routledge companion to environmental planning. London: Routledge; 2019. [Link]
  34. Morss RE, Mulder KJ, Lazo JK, Demuth JL. How do people perceive, understand, and anticipate responding to flash flood risks and warnings? Results from a public survey in Boulder, Colorado, USA. Journal of Hydrology. 2016; 541:649-64. [DOI:10.1016/j.jhydrol.2015.11.047]
  35. EskandariPour M, Soltaninia S. Analyzing the duration frequency and severity of drought using copula function in the Yazd city. Journal of Water and Climate Change. 2022; 13(1):67-82. [DOI:10.2166/wcc.2021.366]
  36. Graneheim UH, Lundman B. Qualitative content analysis in nursing research: Concepts, procedures and measures to achieve trustworthiness. Nurse Education Today. 2004; 24(2):105-12. [DOI:10.1016/j.nedt.2003.10.001] [PMID]
  37. Guba EG, Lincoln YS. Fourth generation evaluation. California: Sage; 1989. [Link]
  38. Wahlberg AA, Sjoberg L. Risk perception and the media. Journal of Risk Research. 2000; 3(1):31-50. [DOI:10.1080/136698700376699]
  39. Wangshu G, Guanhua W. How influencers marketing motivates consumers’ buying behaviour: A focus group investigation of the impulse buying behaviour via Chinese millennials’ lens. 2020. [Link]
  40. Zentner M, Grandjean D, Scherer KR. Emotions evoked by the sound of music: Characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion (Washington, DC). 2008; 8(4):494-521. [DOI:10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.494] [PMID]
  41. Updegraff JA, Sherman DK, Luyster FS, Mann TL. The effects of message quality and congruency on perceptions of tailored health communications. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2007; 43(2):249-57. [DOI:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.01.007] [PMID] [PMCID]
  42. O’Neill P. Developing a risk communication model to encourage community safety from natural hazards. Sydney: N.S.W: State Emergency Services; 2004. [Link]
Type of Study: Research | Subject: Qualitative
Received: 2024/01/24 | Accepted: 2024/07/7 | Published: 2024/10/1
* Corresponding Author Address: Research Center for Emergency and Disaster Resilience, Red Crescent Society of the Islamic Republic of Iran,fars , Iran

Add your comments about this article : Your username or Email:
CAPTCHA

Send email to the article author


Rights and permissions
Creative Commons License This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.

Designed & Developed by : Yektaweb